News / Science News

    White people’s perceptions of the emotions on black people’s faces are less accurate than their perceptions among other white people



    white people’s perceptions of the emotions on black people’s faces are less accurate. Photo: JD Mason/Unsplash


    Scientists have shown that white people find it difficult to tell the difference between genuine and false smiles on black people’s faces—between genuine and fake happiness. What is more, they have discovered that this happens because white people do not look black people in the eye, as the former regard the latter as being different to them (part of the so-called ‘outgroup’).

    Being able to accurately identify emotions in others is important for social interaction in general, but particularly so in interracial contexts, which are prone to misinterpretations and misunderstandings. Indeed, when there is limited emotional identification with other people, communication becomes inhibited and, ultimately, this may lead to negative—even tragic—results.

    These acts of violence have a common denominator: people of colour suffering violent attacks at the hands of white people. They illustrate the importance of the impressions that people belonging to different groups form about one another, in terms of the mistrust, fear, or anger, for instance, that sometimes underlie inter-group interactions.

    Other research had previously shown that we perceive the members of the groups to which we feel we belong (the ‘ingroups’) more accurately and in more detail than members of groups to which we do not consider ourselves to belong (the outgroups).

    Indeed, a number of studies have suggested that white people in the United States tend to perceive the expressions of black people as being angrier than ‘white’ facial expressions.

    The research team on this new study therefore wanted to determine whether similar biases occurred in the identification of expressions of positive emotion, such as happiness or joy.

    The scientists conducted six experiments. In all of the experiments, the participants were shown the smiling faces of white or black people, and were asked to rate the level of happiness they perceived in the images. Some faces showed a genuine smile, while others presented forced or fake smiles.

    Thus, the white participants’ discernment was more accurate—in terms of correctly identifying smiles as genuine or false—when the images they were shown were of other white people, compared to when they were shown images of black people.

    No such difference was found in the case of people of colour, who were able to accurately distinguish between expressions of genuine vs. fake happiness, regardless of whether the person smiling was white or black.

    The results of these studies, and of some others currently under development, demonstrate how important it is to pay attention to individuals’ gaze when attempting to explain how we form our impressions of others, as well as to other marked effects shown in the scientific literature on perceptions and impressions of people from other groups.

    We know from previous research that, of the total time we spend, on average, looking at the faces of other people and forming an impression of them, over 40% is devoted to focusing on the eyes in particular.

    By concentrating on a person’s gaze, it’s more likely that we will avoid some of the more harmful effects of the mistrust and lack of identification we feel towards people from groups other than our own. (University of Granada)

    MAY 9, 2019



    YOU MAY ALSO LIKE

    Other than their purity, flaws in Diamonds may be holding the key to a new type of communications with high security. Diamonds were used by researchers to preserve fragile quantum information over long distances.
    Researchers from the University of Granada, University Hospital La Paz (Madrid) and the University of Texas (USA) have identified a new molecular mechanism underlying the anti-obesity effects of the chronic administration of melatonin, a naturally occurring hormone released by the pineal gland overnight.
    Researchers have identified mutations in a gene called CARD11 that lead to atopic dermatitis, or eczema, an allergic skin disease.
    Moderately reducing caloric intake over a period of two years significantly improved cardiometabolic risk factors in young and middle-aged, non-obese adults, according to new findings from the Comprehensive Assessment of Long-term Effects of Reducing Intake of Energy.
    Children of obese parents may be at risk for developmental delays, according to a study by researchers at the National Institutes of Health.
    Russian scientists believe that terrestrial bacteria that were sent to space and returned to earth now pose a threat to the existence of life on Earth.

    © 1991-2023 The Titi Tudorancea Bulletin | Titi Tudorancea® is a Registered Trademark | Terms of use and privacy policy
    Contact